The Times Australia
The Times World News

.

People use mental shortcuts to make difficult decisions – even highly trained doctors delivering babies

  • Written by Manasvini Singh, Assistant Professor of Health Economics, University of Massachusetts Amherst
People use mental shortcuts to make difficult decisions – even highly trained doctors delivering babies

Being a physician is a difficult job. They must make complex, high-stakes decisions under severe pressure, with limited information about the patient, the disease and the treatment, while juggling personal and hospital priorities under the ever-present threat of lawsuits.

So what do physicians do in such highly uncertain situations?

Like all human beings, they unconsciously rely on quick rules that simplify complex decisions. Psychologists and economists call these mental shortcuts “heuristics[1].”

For example, if your sandwich falls on the floor, you might employ the five-second rule[2] to decide whether to pick it up and eat it or simply throw it away. That’s a heuristic – it allows you to approximate the correct decision quickly and easily, without getting mired in a lengthy mental debate about the pros and cons of each possible course of action.

While the average person’s reliance on heuristics is usually of little concern to society, the use of heuristics by physicians can have serious consequences.

Heuristics in the delivery room

I’m a health economist[3] interested in the intersection of applied decision theory and health care.

There are all kinds of decisions a doctor must make while attending a birth: Should a woman continue to labor if the baby shows signs of distress? What interventions are warranted? Is it time for an emergency cesarean? The physician is responsible for life-and-death choices in a fraught, emotional environment.

In my recent research[4] published in the journal Science, I found that physicians use heuristics in the delivery room in ways that could potentially harm the mother and baby.

Looking at two academic hospitals’ data from more than 86,000 deliveries over 21 years, I saw that physicians who experienced complications during one patient’s delivery were more likely to switch to the other mode of delivery for their next patient, regardless of what the situation calls for. For example, if the physician’s last patient hemorrhaged during her vaginal delivery, the physician is more likely to perform a cesarean delivery for their next patient, even if a C-section is not indicated for that patient.

preparing for operation in darkened surgical suite
A doctor may lean toward a C-section because the last vaginal delivery had complications. SDI Productions/E+ via Getty Images[5]

It appears physicians may overcorrect after a bad outcome, tending to shy away from the decision they believe caused it – even when faced with a new patient with her own unique circumstances.

Complications during a vaginal delivery increased the likelihood of a subsequent C-section by up to 3.6%. That’s about 23 potentially inappropriate C-sections per year per hospital. Complications during a cesarean increased the likelihood of a subsequent vaginal delivery by up to 3.4%. That’s about 50 potentially inappropriate vaginal deliveries per year per hospital.

It’s a sizable effect, considering the baseline effect should be zero. And patients at poorly resourced hospitals that have higher numbers of labor-and-delivery complications are more likely to be affected – as physicians experience more difficulties, this heuristic means they’ll be swayed toward more potentially inappropriate delivery choices.

There is evidence that this switching heuristic is harmful to the affected patient. For instance, if the physician switches delivery modes after the prior delivery had complications, my analysis found that the second patient and/or her baby are more likely to die than if the physician had switched delivery modes after no prior complications.

What’s behind the overcorrection

Since psychologists Amos Tversky[6] and Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman[7] introduced the idea of heuristics and biases[8] into the mainstream a few decades ago, researchers have conducted hundreds of studies establishing the various types of heuristics people rely on in various contexts. While these mental shortcuts are often useful for making immediate judgments with limited information, they can lead people to make very predictable mistakes.

There are several heuristics that could explain the switching behavior I identified in the delivery room data.

Take, for instance, the “win-stay/lose-shift” heuristic, which has been seen in birds[9], bees[10], rats[11], monkeys[12], children[13] and adults[14]. According to this heuristic, individuals stick with a strategy until they experience a “loss,” such as a labor-and-delivery complication. At that point, they switch strategies – like trying a different delivery mode.

Researchers have been especially interested in how experts use heuristics[15], since it is not immediately clear whether people with enhanced knowledge of their specialized fields fall prey to the same decision-making flaws that afflict the lay individual. There is growing evidence that experts in a variety of fields – such as forensic scientists[16], real estate agents[17], elite athletes[18], judges[19], academics[20] and physicians[21] – do, in fact, rely on heuristics. Whether the use of such heuristics leads to poor outcomes – whether it can be called a “bias” – is still a matter of debate.

Useful time-saver or dangerous bias?

A bias arising from a heuristic implies a deviation from an “optimal” decision. However, identifying the optimal decision in real life is difficult because you usually don’t know what could have been: the counterfactual. This is especially relevant in medicine.

Take the win-stay/lose-shift strategy, for example. There are other studies that show that after “bad” events, physicians switch strategies. Missing an important diagnosis makes physicians test more on subsequent patients[22]. Experiencing complications with a drug makes the physician less likely to prescribe it again[23].

But from a learning perspective, it’s difficult to say that ordering a test after missing a diagnosis is a flawed heuristic. Ordering a test always increases the chance that the physician catches an important diagnosis. So it’s a useful heuristic in some instances – say, for example, the physician had been underordering tests before, or the patient or insurer prefers shelling out the extra money for the chance to detect a cancer early.

In my study, though, switching delivery modes after complications offers no documented guarantees of avoiding future complications. And there is the added consideration of the short- and long-term health consequences of delivery-mode choice[24] for mother and baby. Further, people are generally less tolerant of having inappropriate medical procedures performed on them than they are of being the recipients of unnecessary tests.

doctor in scrubs using tablet Decision support can be built into the systems doctors use as they make choices about care. shapecharge/E+ via Getty Images[25]

Tweaking the heuristic

Can physicians’ reliance on heuristics be lessened? Possibly.

Decision support systems[26] that assist physicians with important clinical decisions are gathering momentum in medicine, and could help doctors course-correct after emotional events such as delivery complications.

For example, such algorithms can be built into electronic health records and perform a variety of tasks: flag physician decisions that appear nonstandard, identify patients who could benefit from a particular decision, summarize clinical information in ways that make it easier for physicians to digest and so on. As long as physicians retain at least some autonomy[27], decision support systems can do just that – support doctors in making clinical decisions.

Nudges[28] that unobtrusively encourage physicians to make certain decisions can be accomplished by tinkering with the way options are presented – what’s called “choice architecture.” They already work for other clinical decisions[29].

Imagine a policy objective is to reduce prescription of drug X. The medical record system could present drug X as the last option in the physician’s drop-down menu, or auto-populate a default drug Y that the physician could choose to override. The physician would still be able to prescribe drug X, but it would require a little more mental involvement on their part to do so.

However, it is critical to understand that physicians frequently make highly consequential decisions under immense pressure. Any administrative barriers that hinder their ability to respond to clinical information in real time might harm patients even more. Designing and implementing interventions aimed at improving physician decision-making will be a challenge.

[Get the best of The Conversation, every weekend. Sign up for our weekly newsletter[30].]

References

  1. ^ heuristics (thedecisionlab.com)
  2. ^ five-second rule (theconversation.com)
  3. ^ I’m a health economist (scholar.google.com)
  4. ^ In my recent research (doi.org)
  5. ^ SDI Productions/E+ via Getty Images (www.gettyimages.com)
  6. ^ Amos Tversky (news.stanford.edu)
  7. ^ Daniel Kahneman (scholar.google.com)
  8. ^ introduced the idea of heuristics and biases (doi.org)
  9. ^ birds (doi.org)
  10. ^ bees (doi.org)
  11. ^ rats (doi.org)
  12. ^ monkeys (doi.org)
  13. ^ children (doi.org)
  14. ^ adults (www.pnas.org)
  15. ^ how experts use heuristics (doi.org)
  16. ^ forensic scientists (doi.org)
  17. ^ real estate agents (doi.org)
  18. ^ elite athletes (doi.org)
  19. ^ judges (doi.org)
  20. ^ academics (doi.org)
  21. ^ physicians (pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov)
  22. ^ subsequent patients (doi.org)
  23. ^ less likely to prescribe it again (doi.org)
  24. ^ health consequences of delivery-mode choice (doi.org)
  25. ^ shapecharge/E+ via Getty Images (www.gettyimages.com)
  26. ^ Decision support systems (hbr.org)
  27. ^ some autonomy (doi.org)
  28. ^ Nudges (dx.doi.org)
  29. ^ other clinical decisions (doi.org)
  30. ^ Sign up for our weekly newsletter (theconversation.com)

Read more https://theconversation.com/people-use-mental-shortcuts-to-make-difficult-decisions-even-highly-trained-doctors-delivering-babies-168711

Times Magazine

When Touchscreens Turn Temperamental: What to Do Before You Panic

When your touchscreen starts acting up, ignoring taps, registering phantom touches, or freezing entirely, it can feel like your entire setup is falling apart. Before you rush to replace the device, it’s worth taking a deep breath and exploring what c...

Why Social Media Marketing Matters for Businesses in Australia

Today social media is a big part of daily life. All over Australia people use Facebook, Instagram, TikTok , LinkedIn and Twitter to stay connected, share updates and find new ideas. For businesses this means a great chance to reach new customers and...

Building an AI-First Culture in Your Company

AI isn't just something to think about anymore - it's becoming part of how we live and work, whether we like it or not. At the office, it definitely helps us move faster. But here's the thing: just using tools like ChatGPT or plugging AI into your wo...

Data Management Isn't Just About Tech—Here’s Why It’s a Human Problem Too

Photo by Kevin Kuby Manuel O. Diaz Jr.We live in a world drowning in data. Every click, swipe, medical scan, and financial transaction generates information, so much that managing it all has become one of the biggest challenges of our digital age. Bu...

Headless CMS in Digital Twins and 3D Product Experiences

Image by freepik As the metaverse becomes more advanced and accessible, it's clear that multiple sectors will use digital twins and 3D product experiences to visualize, connect, and streamline efforts better. A digital twin is a virtual replica of ...

The Decline of Hyper-Casual: How Mid-Core Mobile Games Took Over in 2025

In recent years, the mobile gaming landscape has undergone a significant transformation, with mid-core mobile games emerging as the dominant force in app stores by 2025. This shift is underpinned by changing user habits and evolving monetization tr...

The Times Features

Why Mobile Allied Therapy Services Are Essential in Post-Hospital Recovery

Mobile allied health services matter more than ever under recent NDIA travel funding cuts. A quiet but critical shift is unfolding in Australia’s healthcare landscape. Mobile all...

Sydney Fertility Specialist – Expert IVF Treatment for Your Parenthood Journey

Improving the world with the help of a new child is the most valuable dream of many couples. To the infertile, though, this process can be daunting. It is here that a Sydney Fertil...

Could we one day get vaccinated against the gastro bug norovirus? Here’s where scientists are at

Norovirus is the leading cause[1] of acute gastroenteritis outbreaks worldwide. It’s responsible for roughly one in every five cases[2] of gastro annually. Sometimes dubbed ...

Does running ruin your knees? And how old is too old to start?

You’ve probably heard that running is tough on your knees – and even that it can cause long-term damage. But is this true? Running is a relatively high-impact activity. Eve...

Jetstar announces first ever Brisbane to Rarotonga flights with launch fares from just $249^ one-way

Jetstar will start operating direct flights between Brisbane and Rarotonga, the stunning capital island of the Cook Islands, in May 2026, with launch sale fares available today...

Introducing the SE 2 and Mini hair dryers from Laifen

The Mane Attractions for Professional Styling at Home Without the Price Tag Fast, flawless hair is now possible with the launch of Laifen’s two professional quality hair dryers th...